The 
Mathematical Brain
 
Get Acrobat Reader
  
What's New
  
  
Test and New Chapter
 
About Brian Butterworth
 
Reviews
 
email author
 
Interviews
 
MP3 
Audio 
Welcome
 
Get QuickTime 
4
 
Buy Now @ Amazon, UK
 

Macmillan Logo
 


What
Counts
 

Italian Edition
 

Swedish Edition
  

Naze sugaku ga tokui na hito to nigate na hito ga 
irunoka?
(Why are some people good, but others bad at maths?)
 


The 
Mathematical Brain
 
Brian Butterworth
 
 

The Guardian Logo
  


Lion's pride in arithmetic
 
by Brian Butterworth
 
The Guardian: Thursday May 6, 1999.
 

Animals can put two and two together and make four - but how? Brian Butterworth sums up the theories

  


In the Serengeti National Park in Tanzania, a lioness is returning at dusk to her pride. Eighteen females, one adult male and seven cubs are waiting a mile away. She hears a roar, one she does not recognise.

It must be an intruder into her territory. Her cubs are safely with her sisters and she is all alone. She hears the roar again. It's the same lion. Should she try to drive off the intruder? It would be an even match - one against one - and it could turn into a real fight. With lions, that could be fatal. Dusk becomes the inky black of night, and she returns silently to the pride.

The following week, again at dusk, she hears roaring 500 metres away: one unfamiliar voice, then a chorus of roars, one overlapping the next, and none of them the familiar voices. There are three intruders. This time she is with four of her sisters from the pride. Three of them, five of us. Their ears prick. The roaring is coming from a stand of trees over to their left. They wait, they peer into the night, and at each other. One of them, the leader, approaches the roaring cautiously at first, as the others join, more quickly, until they are charging headlong into the trees.

By the time they reach the trees, the roaring has stopped and there is no sign of the intruding lions. The roaring was from a loudspeaker set up by Karen McComb of the University of Sussex and her colleagues. She was testing a theory about the way lions make a numerical assessment of the threat from intruders and the strength of the defence forces.

The theory predicts that lions (like many other animals where fighting would be costly) will contest resources only when they are very likely to win; otherwise they will withdraw. The one exception is when a lioness is with her cub. Then she will always attack an intruder. The lions' decision to attack depends on the number of intruders - in this experiment one or three - and how many adult defenders there are. The lioness leader identifies roaring as coming from individuals who are not members of the pride; she will also represent the defenders as known individuals. The best explanation of her decision is that she enumerates the number of distinguishable roarers and the number of her sisters and compares the two numbers.

Only when the number of defenders is greater than the number of intruders, will she launch the attack. This is remarkable because the number of intruders comes from the sound they make (they are not visible), while the number of defenders comes from another sense or other senses, vision probably, and is stored in the lioness's memory. Thus she has to abstract the number of the two collections - intruders and defenders - away from the sense in which they were experienced then compare these abstracted numbers.

Research on animal counting is helping us understand how we humans understand numbers, one of the key abilities that has taken us from the stone age to the phone age. We process about 16,000 separate numbers a day - dates, times, page numbers, calculations, government statistics, prices, addresses, and phone numbers. That's if we don't work in a bank or a shop, or in a lab. Then of course, we will process vastly more.

Where does this ability come from? Is it just another thing we learn, or fail to learn, at school? One very influential idea comes from the Swiss psychologist Jean Piaget. He argued that our very idea of number is constructed out of previously developed logical abilities. One of these was transitive reasoning: if A is bigger than B, and C is smaller than B, which is the biggest? If you can't figure that our correctly, then you won't be able to put the numbers in order, he claimed. But these logical abilities don't develop until at least four years of age, and are not functioning in their most abstract form until the teens. The Serengeti lions pose a problem here: they certainly cannot reason as well as four year old humans, and yet they can order two numbers as if their lives depended on it.

In the laboratory, where it's possible to ensure that number is the critical variable, all kinds of animals can be trained to make choices based on numbers. Last year two American scientists, Elizabeth Brannon and Herb Terrace, showed that it was possible to train a rhesus monkey to select one of two pictures according to which had the most coloured shapes in it. Back in the forties, the great German ethologist, Otto Koehler, successfully trained a raven called Jakob to open boxes and eat the seeds contained in them until a precise number of seeds had been eaten, and then to stop.

These studies suggest that animals are born with numerical abilities, that can be detected reliably in the lab, and serve a useful purpose in the wild. If we have inherited these abilities from our non-human ancestors, then there should be some evidence of it in children much younger than Piaget's four year olds. Indeed there is. Babies, even newborns, look longer at a display when the number of things in it changes.

Of course, they may be responding to something other than number, but many experiments have now shown that change of number alone can trigger increased looking, and is in fact a more powerful trigger than changing the colour or shape of the objects.

Many different types of evidence are beginning to converge on the theory that we humans are born with brain circuits specialised for numbers. We even have a good idea of where these circuits are in the brain. Have we inherited these brain circuits from our non-human ancestors? Do our genes carry a code for building the same kinds of circuits as the lions, the monkeys and birds use, but perhaps a bit bigger and more advanced? To answer that question, we need to know which parts of their brains these animals use; and we don't yet know that.

 


 
 


What's New Preface Interviews email author Author
 


Macmillan Logo